首页> 美国卫生研究院文献>other >Minimal Interference from Possessor Phrases in the Production of Subject-Verb Agreement
【2h】

Minimal Interference from Possessor Phrases in the Production of Subject-Verb Agreement

机译:从主语短语对主语-动词协议产生的干扰最小

代理获取
本网站仅为用户提供外文OA文献查询和代理获取服务,本网站没有原文。下单后我们将采用程序或人工为您竭诚获取高质量的原文,但由于OA文献来源多样且变更频繁,仍可能出现获取不到、文献不完整或与标题不符等情况,如果获取不到我们将提供退款服务。请知悉。

摘要

We explore the language production process by eliciting subject-verb agreement errors. Participants were asked to create complete sentences from sentence beginnings such as The elf's/elves' house with the tiny window/windows and The statue in the elf's/elves' gardens. These are subject noun phrases containing a head noun and controller of agreement (statue) and two nonheads, a “local noun” (window(s)/garden(s)), and a possessor noun (elf's/elves'). Past research has shown that a plural nonhead noun (an “attractor”) within a subject noun phrase triggers the production of verb agreement errors, and further, that the nearer the attractor to the head noun, the greater the interference. This effect can be interpreted in terms of relative hierarchical distance from the head noun, or via a processing window account, which claims that during production, there is a window in which the head and modifying material may be co-active, and an attractor must be active at the same time as the head to give rise to errors. Using possessors attached at different heights within the same window, we are able to empirically distinguish these accounts. Possessors also allow us to explore two additional issues. First, case marking of local nouns has been shown to reduce agreement errors in languages with “rich” inflectional systems, and we explore whether English speakers attend to case. Secondly, formal syntactic analyses differ regarding the structural position of the possessive marker, and we distinguish them empirically with the relative magnitude of errors produced by possessors and local nouns. Our results show that, across the board, plural possessors are significantly less disruptive to the agreement process than plural local nouns. Proximity to the head noun matters: a possessor directly modifying the head noun induce a significant number of errors, but a possessor within a modifying prepositional phrase did not, though the local noun did. These findings suggest that proximity to a head noun is independent of a “processing window” effect. They also support a noun phrase-internal, case-like analysis of the structural position of the possessive ending and show that even speakers of inflectionally impoverished languages like English are sensitive to morphophonological case-like marking.
机译:我们通过引起主语-动词一致错误来探索语言的产生过程。要求参与者从句子开头创建完整的句子,例如带有小窗户的小精灵/小精灵的房子和小精灵/小精灵的花园中的雕像。这些是主题名词短语,包含主名词和协议控制人(雕像)和两个非主名词,“局部名词”(窗口/花园)和拥有者名词(“精灵” /“精灵”)。过去的研究表明,主语名词短语中的多个非头部名词(“吸引者”)会触发动词一致性错误的产生,此外,吸引子离头部名词越近,干扰越大。可以用与头部名词的相对等级距离或通过处理窗口帐户来解释这种效果,该帐户声称在生产过程中存在一个窗口,在该窗口中头部和修饰材料可以共同起作用,并且吸引子必须与头部同时活动,以引起错误。使用在同一窗口内以不同高度连接的所有物,我们可以凭经验区分这些帐户。拥有者还使我们能够探索另外两个问题。首先,已经证明了使用局部名词的大小写标记可以减少带有“丰富”变形系统的语言的一致错误,并且我们探索英语使用者是否关注大小写。其次,形式所有句法的分析在所有格标记的结构位置上有所不同,并且我们根据所有格和局部名词产生的错误的相对大小从经验上区分它们。我们的结果表明,总体而言,复数所有者比复数本地名词对协议过程的破坏要小得多。接近头部名词很重要:直接修饰头部名词的拥有者会引起大量错误,但修饰介词短语中的拥有者却不会,尽管本地名词会引起错误。这些发现表明,接近首名词不依赖于“处理窗口”效应。他们还支持名词性短语的内部使用,对所有格结尾的结构位置进行大小写分析,并表明,即使是像英语这样的语境严重贫困的语言使用者也对形态学上的大小写敏感。

著录项

相似文献

  • 外文文献
  • 中文文献
  • 专利
代理获取

客服邮箱:kefu@zhangqiaokeyan.com

京公网安备:11010802029741号 ICP备案号:京ICP备15016152号-6 六维联合信息科技 (北京) 有限公司©版权所有
  • 客服微信

  • 服务号