首页> 外文学位 >Contributions from practical problem-solving ability, memory locus of control, and memory self-efficacy to memory performance for American-born and Russian-speaking elderly.
【24h】

Contributions from practical problem-solving ability, memory locus of control, and memory self-efficacy to memory performance for American-born and Russian-speaking elderly.

机译:实际解决问题的能力,记忆的控制源和记忆的自我效能感对美国出生和说俄语的老人的记忆表现的贡献。

获取原文
获取原文并翻译 | 示例

摘要

The present study examined the contributions of everyday problem-solving styles, memory locus of control, memory self-efficacy, and culture in predicting memory performance in Russian-speaking and American-born elderly. Memory locus of control and everyday problem-solving styles have not been investigated in relation to memory performance, cross-culturally or otherwise. Thirty American-born (age M = 72.10) and 30 Russian-speaking older adults (age M = 77.60) completed the demographic questionnaire, the Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test, the Memory Self-Efficacy Questionnaire (Berry, West, & Dennehey, 1989), the Capacity and the Locus scales from the Metamemory In Adulthood (Dixon & Hultsch, 1983), the Everyday Problem Solving Inventory (Cornelius & Caspi, 1987), the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al., 1961), and the Schwartz Value Survey (Schwartz, 1992). Multivariate analyses of covariance were performed to test for the differences in means between groups. Although there were no significant differences on the individualistic/collectivist dimension, the Russians rated conformity values as significantly more important than the Americans. The Americans rated the self-direction values as significantly more important than the Russians. Both, the Russians and the Americans showed a significant trend for utilizing combined problem focused action and cognitive problem analysis approaches (AB) in problem-solving. For the Americans, results indicated a significant negative relationship between the combined passive-dependent and an avoidant thinking and denial approaches (CD) and memory performance and for the Russians, a significant positive relationship between the approaches AB and memory performance. None of the approaches were correlated with memory self-efficacy and memory locus of control. The Russians' memory locus of control was more external and their memory self-efficacy was lower than that of the Americans'. Although the Russians scored in a significantly depressed range on the BDI (M = 17.17, SD = 7.33), it did not impact on any of the dependent variables. For both groups, there were significant relationships in the expected direction between the value ratings and dependent variables. In sum, results demonstrated that culture had an impact on dependent variables investigated in this study. Researchers and clinicians working with this population may want to consider cultural differences in personality characteristics and their impact on day-to-day functioning.
机译:本研究检查了日常解决问题的方式,控制记忆的轨迹,记忆的自我效能感和文化在预测说俄语和美国出生的老年人的记忆表现方面的作用。尚未针对跨文化或其他方面的记忆表现研究过记忆的控制源和日常问题解决方式。 30名美国出生的婴儿( M = 72.10)和30位讲俄语的老年人(年龄 M = 77.60)完成了人口统计学调查问卷,Rivermead行为记忆测试,记忆自我效能感调查表(Berry,West和Dennehey,1989年),能力和所在地点来自成年后的元记忆(Dixon和Hultsch,1983年),日常问题解决量表(Cornelius和Caspi,1987年),贝克抑郁症库存(Beck等,1961)和施瓦兹价值调查(Schwartz,1992)。进行协方差的多变量分析以检验组之间均值的差异。尽管在个人主义/集体主义方面没有显着差异,但俄罗斯人认为合格值比美国人重要得多。美国人认为自我指导值比俄国人重要得多。俄国人和美国人都显示出在解决问题中结合使用针对问题的行动和认知问题分析方法(AB)的重要趋势。对于美国人而言,结果表明,被动依赖和回避思维与否定方法(CD)与记忆力之间存在显着的负相关关系;对俄罗斯人而言,AB和记忆力之间存在显着的正相关关系。这些方法都没有与记忆自我效能和控制的记忆位点相关。俄国人的记忆控制力源于外部,其记忆自我效能感比美国人低。尽管俄罗斯人在BDI上的得分明显偏低(M = 17.17,SD = 7.33),但它并未影响任何因变量。对于这两个组,价值评级和因变量之间在预期方向上都存在显着关系。总之,结果表明文化对本研究中调查的因变量有影响。与该人群一起工作的研究人员和临床医生可能希望考虑人格特征的文化差异及其对日常功能的影响。

著录项

相似文献

  • 外文文献
  • 中文文献
  • 专利
获取原文

客服邮箱:kefu@zhangqiaokeyan.com

京公网安备:11010802029741号 ICP备案号:京ICP备15016152号-6 六维联合信息科技 (北京) 有限公司©版权所有
  • 客服微信

  • 服务号